Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats

G. Andrew Mickley, Melanie A. Schaldach, Kristyn J. Snyder, Seth A. Balogh, Traci Len, Kevin Neimanis, Paula Goulis, Jennifer Hug, Kimberly Sauchak, Dawn R. Remmers-Roeber, Crystal Walker, Bryan Yamamoto

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

40 Citations (Scopus)

Abstract

These experiments explored the effects of glutamate, N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor blockade on the formation, retention, and expression of conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in young rats. Previous data from our laboratory suggested that ketamine administration potentiates a CTA in E18 rat fetuses. The current studies investigated this phenomenon in neonates. High-pressure liquid chromatography (HPLC) methods were used to determine the amount of ketamine that must be injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) to achieve brain ketamine levels in neonates comparable to those found in the fetuses from our previous experiments. Then, on their day of birth, Sprague-Dawley rat pups received injections of either 0.1, 10, or 70 mg/kg of ketamine HCl, i.p. or a Sal control injection. One-half hour later, pups were injected orally with either Saccharin (Sac; 10 μL of 0.3%) or water followed by an injection of either lithium chloride (LiCl; 81 mg/kg) or Sal (i.p.). The CTA was evaluated in two different tests. Two weeks after conditioning, the dam was anesthetized and the frequency with which pups attached to Sac-painted nipples versus nipples painted with water was measured (i.e., the nipple taste test, NTT). Controls for state-dependent learning were run in which 10 mg/kg of ketamine or saline (Sal) was administered before both taste aversion conditioning and the NTT. After weaning, the CTA was also evaluated by measuring the amount of Sac (0.3%) or water consumed during a two-bottle test. Neonates that received Sal control injections before the Sac + LiCl pairing acquired CTAs and avoided Sac-painted nipples. However, the pups injected with ketamine on the conditioning day only (P0) did not avoid Sac-painted nipples (as compared to controls). Pups that had ketamine both at the time of CTA training and testing, or just before the NTT, also failed to avoid Sac-painted nipples. Ketamine's acute effects apparently influenced the outcome of the NTT of state-dependent control subjects. Rat pups that received the highest doses of ketamine (10 or 70 mg/kg) and tasted Sac on P0 later failed to show a neophobia for Sac-painted nipples. Whereas, rat pups that received the high dose of ketamine and water on P0, later exhibited a neophobic response. These data suggest that ketamine did not impair the animal's ability to taste Sac. These data reflecting a ketamine-induced blockade of neonatal CTAs may be contrasted with our previous findings in which ketamine potentiated fetal CTAs. However, they are in consonance with data from adult rats suggesting that ketamine can cause an amnesia for CTAs. NMDA receptor blockade may shape memory formation in a manner that is dependent on the stage of brain development. Copyright (C) 1998 Elsevier Science Inc.

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)381-390
Number of pages10
JournalPhysiology and Behavior
Volume64
Issue number3
DOIs
StatePublished - Jun 1 1998
Externally publishedYes

Fingerprint

Ketamine
Nipples
Injections
Water
Rat
Aversion
N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptors
Fetus
Lithium Chloride
Saccharin
Aptitude
Amnesia
Brain
Weaning
Sprague Dawley Rats
Glutamic Acid

Keywords

  • Conditioned Taste Aversion
  • Development
  • Glutamate
  • Ketamine
  • Learning
  • Lithium Chloride
  • Neonate
  • Neural Plasticity
  • NMDA Antagonism
  • NMDA receptors
  • Saccharin

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Experimental and Cognitive Psychology
  • Philosophy
  • Behavioral Neuroscience

Cite this

Mickley, G. A., Schaldach, M. A., Snyder, K. J., Balogh, S. A., Len, T., Neimanis, K., ... Yamamoto, B. (1998). Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats. Physiology and Behavior, 64(3), 381-390. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00097-3

Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats. / Mickley, G. Andrew; Schaldach, Melanie A.; Snyder, Kristyn J.; Balogh, Seth A.; Len, Traci; Neimanis, Kevin; Goulis, Paula; Hug, Jennifer; Sauchak, Kimberly; Remmers-Roeber, Dawn R.; Walker, Crystal; Yamamoto, Bryan.

In: Physiology and Behavior, Vol. 64, No. 3, 01.06.1998, p. 381-390.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Mickley, GA, Schaldach, MA, Snyder, KJ, Balogh, SA, Len, T, Neimanis, K, Goulis, P, Hug, J, Sauchak, K, Remmers-Roeber, DR, Walker, C & Yamamoto, B 1998, 'Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats', Physiology and Behavior, vol. 64, no. 3, pp. 381-390. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00097-3
Mickley GA, Schaldach MA, Snyder KJ, Balogh SA, Len T, Neimanis K et al. Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats. Physiology and Behavior. 1998 Jun 1;64(3):381-390. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00097-3
Mickley, G. Andrew ; Schaldach, Melanie A. ; Snyder, Kristyn J. ; Balogh, Seth A. ; Len, Traci ; Neimanis, Kevin ; Goulis, Paula ; Hug, Jennifer ; Sauchak, Kimberly ; Remmers-Roeber, Dawn R. ; Walker, Crystal ; Yamamoto, Bryan. / Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats. In: Physiology and Behavior. 1998 ; Vol. 64, No. 3. pp. 381-390.
@article{1dc0bb44eb6841389ada5775f8d18659,
title = "Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats",
abstract = "These experiments explored the effects of glutamate, N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor blockade on the formation, retention, and expression of conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in young rats. Previous data from our laboratory suggested that ketamine administration potentiates a CTA in E18 rat fetuses. The current studies investigated this phenomenon in neonates. High-pressure liquid chromatography (HPLC) methods were used to determine the amount of ketamine that must be injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) to achieve brain ketamine levels in neonates comparable to those found in the fetuses from our previous experiments. Then, on their day of birth, Sprague-Dawley rat pups received injections of either 0.1, 10, or 70 mg/kg of ketamine HCl, i.p. or a Sal control injection. One-half hour later, pups were injected orally with either Saccharin (Sac; 10 μL of 0.3{\%}) or water followed by an injection of either lithium chloride (LiCl; 81 mg/kg) or Sal (i.p.). The CTA was evaluated in two different tests. Two weeks after conditioning, the dam was anesthetized and the frequency with which pups attached to Sac-painted nipples versus nipples painted with water was measured (i.e., the nipple taste test, NTT). Controls for state-dependent learning were run in which 10 mg/kg of ketamine or saline (Sal) was administered before both taste aversion conditioning and the NTT. After weaning, the CTA was also evaluated by measuring the amount of Sac (0.3{\%}) or water consumed during a two-bottle test. Neonates that received Sal control injections before the Sac + LiCl pairing acquired CTAs and avoided Sac-painted nipples. However, the pups injected with ketamine on the conditioning day only (P0) did not avoid Sac-painted nipples (as compared to controls). Pups that had ketamine both at the time of CTA training and testing, or just before the NTT, also failed to avoid Sac-painted nipples. Ketamine's acute effects apparently influenced the outcome of the NTT of state-dependent control subjects. Rat pups that received the highest doses of ketamine (10 or 70 mg/kg) and tasted Sac on P0 later failed to show a neophobia for Sac-painted nipples. Whereas, rat pups that received the high dose of ketamine and water on P0, later exhibited a neophobic response. These data suggest that ketamine did not impair the animal's ability to taste Sac. These data reflecting a ketamine-induced blockade of neonatal CTAs may be contrasted with our previous findings in which ketamine potentiated fetal CTAs. However, they are in consonance with data from adult rats suggesting that ketamine can cause an amnesia for CTAs. NMDA receptor blockade may shape memory formation in a manner that is dependent on the stage of brain development. Copyright (C) 1998 Elsevier Science Inc.",
keywords = "Conditioned Taste Aversion, Development, Glutamate, Ketamine, Learning, Lithium Chloride, Neonate, Neural Plasticity, NMDA Antagonism, NMDA receptors, Saccharin",
author = "Mickley, {G. Andrew} and Schaldach, {Melanie A.} and Snyder, {Kristyn J.} and Balogh, {Seth A.} and Traci Len and Kevin Neimanis and Paula Goulis and Jennifer Hug and Kimberly Sauchak and Remmers-Roeber, {Dawn R.} and Crystal Walker and Bryan Yamamoto",
year = "1998",
month = "6",
day = "1",
doi = "10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00097-3",
language = "English (US)",
volume = "64",
pages = "381--390",
journal = "Physiology and Behavior",
issn = "0031-9384",
publisher = "Elsevier Inc.",
number = "3",

}

TY - JOUR

T1 - Ketamine blocks a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in neonatal rats

AU - Mickley, G. Andrew

AU - Schaldach, Melanie A.

AU - Snyder, Kristyn J.

AU - Balogh, Seth A.

AU - Len, Traci

AU - Neimanis, Kevin

AU - Goulis, Paula

AU - Hug, Jennifer

AU - Sauchak, Kimberly

AU - Remmers-Roeber, Dawn R.

AU - Walker, Crystal

AU - Yamamoto, Bryan

PY - 1998/6/1

Y1 - 1998/6/1

N2 - These experiments explored the effects of glutamate, N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor blockade on the formation, retention, and expression of conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in young rats. Previous data from our laboratory suggested that ketamine administration potentiates a CTA in E18 rat fetuses. The current studies investigated this phenomenon in neonates. High-pressure liquid chromatography (HPLC) methods were used to determine the amount of ketamine that must be injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) to achieve brain ketamine levels in neonates comparable to those found in the fetuses from our previous experiments. Then, on their day of birth, Sprague-Dawley rat pups received injections of either 0.1, 10, or 70 mg/kg of ketamine HCl, i.p. or a Sal control injection. One-half hour later, pups were injected orally with either Saccharin (Sac; 10 μL of 0.3%) or water followed by an injection of either lithium chloride (LiCl; 81 mg/kg) or Sal (i.p.). The CTA was evaluated in two different tests. Two weeks after conditioning, the dam was anesthetized and the frequency with which pups attached to Sac-painted nipples versus nipples painted with water was measured (i.e., the nipple taste test, NTT). Controls for state-dependent learning were run in which 10 mg/kg of ketamine or saline (Sal) was administered before both taste aversion conditioning and the NTT. After weaning, the CTA was also evaluated by measuring the amount of Sac (0.3%) or water consumed during a two-bottle test. Neonates that received Sal control injections before the Sac + LiCl pairing acquired CTAs and avoided Sac-painted nipples. However, the pups injected with ketamine on the conditioning day only (P0) did not avoid Sac-painted nipples (as compared to controls). Pups that had ketamine both at the time of CTA training and testing, or just before the NTT, also failed to avoid Sac-painted nipples. Ketamine's acute effects apparently influenced the outcome of the NTT of state-dependent control subjects. Rat pups that received the highest doses of ketamine (10 or 70 mg/kg) and tasted Sac on P0 later failed to show a neophobia for Sac-painted nipples. Whereas, rat pups that received the high dose of ketamine and water on P0, later exhibited a neophobic response. These data suggest that ketamine did not impair the animal's ability to taste Sac. These data reflecting a ketamine-induced blockade of neonatal CTAs may be contrasted with our previous findings in which ketamine potentiated fetal CTAs. However, they are in consonance with data from adult rats suggesting that ketamine can cause an amnesia for CTAs. NMDA receptor blockade may shape memory formation in a manner that is dependent on the stage of brain development. Copyright (C) 1998 Elsevier Science Inc.

AB - These experiments explored the effects of glutamate, N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor blockade on the formation, retention, and expression of conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in young rats. Previous data from our laboratory suggested that ketamine administration potentiates a CTA in E18 rat fetuses. The current studies investigated this phenomenon in neonates. High-pressure liquid chromatography (HPLC) methods were used to determine the amount of ketamine that must be injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) to achieve brain ketamine levels in neonates comparable to those found in the fetuses from our previous experiments. Then, on their day of birth, Sprague-Dawley rat pups received injections of either 0.1, 10, or 70 mg/kg of ketamine HCl, i.p. or a Sal control injection. One-half hour later, pups were injected orally with either Saccharin (Sac; 10 μL of 0.3%) or water followed by an injection of either lithium chloride (LiCl; 81 mg/kg) or Sal (i.p.). The CTA was evaluated in two different tests. Two weeks after conditioning, the dam was anesthetized and the frequency with which pups attached to Sac-painted nipples versus nipples painted with water was measured (i.e., the nipple taste test, NTT). Controls for state-dependent learning were run in which 10 mg/kg of ketamine or saline (Sal) was administered before both taste aversion conditioning and the NTT. After weaning, the CTA was also evaluated by measuring the amount of Sac (0.3%) or water consumed during a two-bottle test. Neonates that received Sal control injections before the Sac + LiCl pairing acquired CTAs and avoided Sac-painted nipples. However, the pups injected with ketamine on the conditioning day only (P0) did not avoid Sac-painted nipples (as compared to controls). Pups that had ketamine both at the time of CTA training and testing, or just before the NTT, also failed to avoid Sac-painted nipples. Ketamine's acute effects apparently influenced the outcome of the NTT of state-dependent control subjects. Rat pups that received the highest doses of ketamine (10 or 70 mg/kg) and tasted Sac on P0 later failed to show a neophobia for Sac-painted nipples. Whereas, rat pups that received the high dose of ketamine and water on P0, later exhibited a neophobic response. These data suggest that ketamine did not impair the animal's ability to taste Sac. These data reflecting a ketamine-induced blockade of neonatal CTAs may be contrasted with our previous findings in which ketamine potentiated fetal CTAs. However, they are in consonance with data from adult rats suggesting that ketamine can cause an amnesia for CTAs. NMDA receptor blockade may shape memory formation in a manner that is dependent on the stage of brain development. Copyright (C) 1998 Elsevier Science Inc.

KW - Conditioned Taste Aversion

KW - Development

KW - Glutamate

KW - Ketamine

KW - Learning

KW - Lithium Chloride

KW - Neonate

KW - Neural Plasticity

KW - NMDA Antagonism

KW - NMDA receptors

KW - Saccharin

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=0032098534&partnerID=8YFLogxK

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=0032098534&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00097-3

DO - 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00097-3

M3 - Article

C2 - 9748108

AN - SCOPUS:0032098534

VL - 64

SP - 381

EP - 390

JO - Physiology and Behavior

JF - Physiology and Behavior

SN - 0031-9384

IS - 3

ER -